1 00:00:11,180 --> 00:00:14,210 Good evening, everybody. Hope you can hear me well. Yes. 2 00:00:14,450 --> 00:00:20,959 Excellent. So I'm as I said, I'm going to talk about scaling, but actually, I'm not going to talk about that type of scaling. 3 00:00:20,960 --> 00:00:26,810 I'm going to talk about dynamical scaling and how we actually use that to be able to understand population dynamics. 4 00:00:27,170 --> 00:00:34,819 As I said in my background, as is in zoology, I'm actually trained as an ecologist, so I'm going to talk a lot about ecology. 5 00:00:34,820 --> 00:00:37,100 But we also, as I said in my group, 6 00:00:37,100 --> 00:00:44,000 we have lots of diverse interests across the breadth of the life sciences where we've been interested in thinking about applying mathematics. 7 00:00:44,300 --> 00:00:46,879 And we're going to I'm going to sort of delve into those sorts of things. 8 00:00:46,880 --> 00:00:52,190 And I'm also going to do a little experiment halfway through or thereabouts where some of you will get to eat chocolate. 9 00:00:52,220 --> 00:00:57,890 So you have to be fast. When I need six volunteers, the people who put their hands up fastest will get fed chocolate. 10 00:00:58,280 --> 00:01:03,350 So I am going to is are we going to do that? And I'm going to start at the very end. And it's another little game that we used to solve. 11 00:01:04,460 --> 00:01:08,060 So I explore some ideas in mathematics and biology. 12 00:01:08,060 --> 00:01:12,020 So this is what I was just saying is very, very, very common in biology. 13 00:01:12,020 --> 00:01:16,430 We think about scaling in in a static way. So here, just like this. 14 00:01:17,910 --> 00:01:21,450 Some sort of crab with a sort of a pincer. 15 00:01:21,690 --> 00:01:28,200 And we can relate it to the size of the body, the size of the crab, to its the size of its pincer. 16 00:01:28,410 --> 00:01:33,810 And often those are plotted on a log scale and they show that this is very classic al symmetric scaling. 17 00:01:33,930 --> 00:01:37,560 And in this case that I just nicked from the web, we can just see there's actually a grey. 18 00:01:37,570 --> 00:01:42,000 The bigger the crab, the much bigger it is. 19 00:01:42,270 --> 00:01:45,540 Pincer is in relation to its body size. 20 00:01:46,620 --> 00:01:51,870 So this idea of scaling in this elementary scaling in biology is very common, but it's a static thing. 21 00:01:51,870 --> 00:01:53,429 We tend just to measure species. 22 00:01:53,430 --> 00:01:58,860 We can go out and we can collect crabs so we can measure the length of the car pieces and we can measure the length of their pincers. 23 00:01:58,860 --> 00:02:02,640 And we can look to see how that how those relationships play out. 24 00:02:02,650 --> 00:02:08,520 Well, I'm a population dynamics artist, and I'm interested in thinking about how things change through time and space. 25 00:02:08,760 --> 00:02:13,319 But tonight, just about moment, mostly about time. And we want to think about how we do that. 26 00:02:13,320 --> 00:02:16,740 How do we scale across these different types of dynamics? 27 00:02:16,740 --> 00:02:24,720 How do we take into account the fact that things may operate on different temporal or spatial time, time or space spatial scales? 28 00:02:25,200 --> 00:02:31,020 So I'm going to go through about three things, three different types of ways that we approach this. 29 00:02:31,350 --> 00:02:35,879 And I'm going to so explain the mouse and I'm going explain the biology behind some of these with some examples. 30 00:02:35,880 --> 00:02:40,920 I'm going to explain some of the maths and at the end I'm going to show and talk about the implications where this becomes really, 31 00:02:40,920 --> 00:02:49,170 really important and why we think it's important and all the stuff that we do in in extending our ideas of maths and biology to the real world. 32 00:02:50,280 --> 00:02:51,959 So I'm going to talk about slow, fast dynamics. 33 00:02:51,960 --> 00:02:55,860 I'm going to talk about this functional response around this point here where we get to surprise interactions. 34 00:02:56,220 --> 00:02:57,420 That's where we get to eat chocolate. 35 00:02:57,750 --> 00:03:03,390 This point here, I'll talk about how we can how we can use approximations, and I'll talk about some stuff that we've been doing. 36 00:03:03,570 --> 00:03:08,670 And so thinking about how we can understand bipolar disorder and mood fluctuations, 37 00:03:08,960 --> 00:03:14,520 I'm going to finish off with some implications and some very contemporary stuff that we do in terms of biological control. 38 00:03:15,570 --> 00:03:19,670 So slow, fast dynamics. What I mean by that, well, I'm going to I'm thinking, okay. 39 00:03:20,610 --> 00:03:24,749 I'm thinking about these sorts of systems. So this is a sign of our regular system. 40 00:03:24,750 --> 00:03:29,459 If any of you ride horses, I know it is about horses. This plant is pretty nasty to horses. 41 00:03:29,460 --> 00:03:35,310 It causes lots of things that make them sick and poorly. It's a very it's a very common plant in the UK landscape. 42 00:03:35,310 --> 00:03:41,430 It's a accident. It's an either an annual short lived perennial plant or long live perennial plant. 43 00:03:41,430 --> 00:03:48,540 It has these very colourful and yellow flowers, but it's stuffed full of very nasty things that may make horses and other things sick. 44 00:03:49,050 --> 00:03:54,420 This is the Cinnabon moth. It's as a as a caterpillar. It feeds on this this particular plant. 45 00:03:54,420 --> 00:04:01,139 It only feeds on this particular plant. And it's able to sequester, capture up those those those chemicals. 46 00:04:01,140 --> 00:04:07,110 And under that, these caterpillars boast that the caterpillars are distasteful and the the moths are distasteful. 47 00:04:07,110 --> 00:04:11,820 This is very pretty. It's on the way out in about the middle of May, through to the end of July. 48 00:04:12,030 --> 00:04:16,679 And it's a pretty common sight, a pretty common moth. We're interested in the dynamics. 49 00:04:16,680 --> 00:04:21,600 How do what sort of population dynamics, how does this moth and plant interact over time? 50 00:04:21,930 --> 00:04:26,670 And one of the things that we can do is we can write down an equation to capture that sort of dynamic. 51 00:04:27,120 --> 00:04:33,029 So so this might be one way that we might do that. So I have resources are changing through time. 52 00:04:33,030 --> 00:04:39,540 So the change in resource through time on the left. And then I've got some of the biology that describes that, that sort of dynamic. 53 00:04:39,540 --> 00:04:43,560 So Lambda is going to be some input rate of resources into the system. 54 00:04:43,950 --> 00:04:47,370 And then I have some consumption by the moths which I. 55 00:04:48,330 --> 00:04:52,680 Usefully called G and they're consuming this resource at right alpha. 56 00:04:53,010 --> 00:04:57,720 And the other thing that will happen is if the resource doesn't get eaten, it will just decay away. 57 00:04:58,470 --> 00:05:01,780 And then there's some coupling and I'll come back to that coupling later in the talk. 58 00:05:01,800 --> 00:05:08,630 But we're going to couple the. So what gets eaten by the what gets eaten by them also gets converted into new, important new moth. 59 00:05:08,640 --> 00:05:14,430 So this is the birth rate of the moths, if you like. And, and then the only thing that's going to happen to them is that going to die. 60 00:05:14,790 --> 00:05:17,099 And we can do lots of analysis on this sort of model. 61 00:05:17,100 --> 00:05:23,759 We can really think about, you know, what, when the population dynamics would be stable, when they might show cycles, I was quite involved. 62 00:05:23,760 --> 00:05:27,360 And we really want to think about, well, actually, if we're thinking about scaling all those things, 63 00:05:27,750 --> 00:05:32,159 all those things operating on the same sort of temporal scales, and if not, 64 00:05:32,160 --> 00:05:36,210 can we get round that other some approximations that we can take to think about that? 65 00:05:38,380 --> 00:05:42,760 So one of the things we can do is we can say, well, actually, maybe the resource dynamics are operating faster. 66 00:05:42,760 --> 00:05:46,360 Maybe this plant is just going through its lifecycle a lot faster than the moth. 67 00:05:46,360 --> 00:05:50,950 And I can't really drop my tummy and pop my head at the same time. But you get the idea this hand is going faster than this. 68 00:05:50,950 --> 00:05:55,840 And so the resource dynamics are going faster than than the moth dynamics. 69 00:05:56,140 --> 00:06:00,190 And the idea then is that the moth only experiences on average what the resources are doing. 70 00:06:00,610 --> 00:06:06,579 So we can then approximate those resource dynamics with a little bit with with a little bit of, with a bit of math. 71 00:06:06,580 --> 00:06:10,750 So we're going to assume that actually, on average, those resource dynamics are not changing. 72 00:06:10,750 --> 00:06:14,170 The D, the d r by d t is set at zero. 73 00:06:15,070 --> 00:06:23,190 So if we do that, I can then sort of set that first equation equal to zero and I can send re-arrange it just algebraically for R. 74 00:06:23,920 --> 00:06:30,190 They're making a couple of assumptions in that and doing that. Firstly, as I said, the resource dynamics are going faster than the math dynamics, 75 00:06:30,190 --> 00:06:35,479 but the little things are going to make an assumption about is the loss rate, this decay rate of the resource. 76 00:06:35,480 --> 00:06:41,350 So we're just going to see that the models are really good eating their resource and not so good and they eat lots of it. 77 00:06:41,350 --> 00:06:47,860 So there's very little left around so that actually the resource consumption rate is actually exceeding these loss rates. 78 00:06:47,860 --> 00:06:54,640 And if we do that, we can make this some very simple approximation. And so if we make this rearrangement to the average, 79 00:06:55,360 --> 00:07:02,739 the average resource dynamics are all now a given by lambda divided by this this the rate of consumption and the number of that around. 80 00:07:02,740 --> 00:07:07,390 And that makes pretty good sense. Basically, if there's lots of resource flowing into the system, 81 00:07:07,690 --> 00:07:16,389 that means the resource is going to increase and as the numbers of of the month increase than the amount of resources going to go down. 82 00:07:16,390 --> 00:07:24,160 So it captures in some and some sense what might happen to the resource up our average amount of resource that's around in the system. 83 00:07:25,300 --> 00:07:26,710 But we're not really interested in that. 84 00:07:26,720 --> 00:07:35,580 I'm trying to think about what would we do when we when we plug that into the to the second equation to do with to do with the to the moth. 85 00:07:35,580 --> 00:07:40,750 So I what I'm going to do is going to replace this term here by the approximate that by that approximation. 86 00:07:41,470 --> 00:07:48,580 So if I do that, I'm going to take this average result some place replace the all in in that second equation. 87 00:07:48,940 --> 00:07:53,259 And it's quite we get we get a much simpler expression in terms of the moth dynamic. 88 00:07:53,260 --> 00:07:59,260 So I can basically say I can cancel these two terms and I'm left with lambda minus new G. 89 00:07:59,350 --> 00:08:06,550 So basically so the. So in this in this slow, fast dynamic, I have the amount of resource flowing into the system. 90 00:08:06,850 --> 00:08:10,330 I know the only thing the other thing that's happening is that the moths are just dying. 91 00:08:10,720 --> 00:08:14,170 Just a nice, simple equation. So nice, simple differential equation. 92 00:08:14,500 --> 00:08:19,250 And on on the right hand side of this and I'm pleasing my schoolteacher very much 93 00:08:19,360 --> 00:08:24,999 because she or she used to tell me that I should show my workings a lot more. What we can do is that we can solve that equation. 94 00:08:25,000 --> 00:08:29,530 I can go from this equation to knowing what was this function actually look like for all time? 95 00:08:29,530 --> 00:08:34,870 So how many generalist moths, how many moths would be around at each point in time? 96 00:08:35,170 --> 00:08:39,340 So I could work through some calculus? Is this this is this equation written small? 97 00:08:39,550 --> 00:08:42,700 And here is my solution. At the end, it's a bit of calculation. 98 00:08:42,700 --> 00:08:48,040 You need to know how to take the integral of one over G and something else and it all falls out. 99 00:08:48,040 --> 00:08:55,719 Basically, I have this sort of function at the end. Okay, so that as well it may mean something to somebody, but we can do some things with this now. 100 00:08:55,720 --> 00:08:59,920 We can I can draw it on the board or we can just do some thought experiments about what 101 00:08:59,920 --> 00:09:05,200 happens for different values of T and the best values of T to pick are zero and infinity. 102 00:09:05,650 --> 00:09:13,720 So if we set t, if we set T equal to zero and we work through this and everything else is just we work through to see what happens. 103 00:09:13,900 --> 00:09:17,110 What how many generations do we expect to be around? At times zero. 104 00:09:17,260 --> 00:09:21,879 It turns out to be zero. So we are down at the origin of a graph on an x y graph. 105 00:09:21,880 --> 00:09:25,810 We're down in that. So when there's no time, there's no generate, there's no models around. 106 00:09:26,980 --> 00:09:28,930 If we set T to infinity, 107 00:09:29,170 --> 00:09:39,790 well basically this then approximate this this term here then is essentially zero and we're left with one -0 lambda over you, lambda over anew. 108 00:09:39,790 --> 00:09:43,179 And what that basically tells me is that there's then there's a fixed state. 109 00:09:43,180 --> 00:09:50,920 So if I know the value of the resource input, which is lambda and I know this decay rate, then I should be able to say how many generations around. 110 00:09:51,250 --> 00:09:57,790 But a very cool thing about that, this approximation is that if we setting t to infinity and infinity goes on forever, 111 00:09:58,060 --> 00:10:04,480 then I go from zero to some sum fixed constant value a ratio between London and a new. 112 00:10:05,530 --> 00:10:09,429 And that's really cool because that's telling me that the dynamic should be pretty stable, right? 113 00:10:09,430 --> 00:10:15,850 So for all time beyond zero days, this graph is going to go up and it's going to stay flat for all time. 114 00:10:15,850 --> 00:10:20,499 And that tells me that actually with this approximation, my slow, fast dynamics, 115 00:10:20,500 --> 00:10:26,860 I should expect to see resource dynamics being the most dynamic, being relatively stable. 116 00:10:27,640 --> 00:10:33,910 Why is that? Well, there's an ecological process, so the resources are flowing into the system and it's called the donor control. 117 00:10:33,910 --> 00:10:35,110 That's a bit of a jargon term, 118 00:10:35,110 --> 00:10:41,830 but it basically means that the resources are flowing into the system and then modulating the dynamics between the moth and the resource. 119 00:10:42,040 --> 00:10:48,060 And they're giving rise to these very stable dynamics. So the next thing we have to ask is, well, how true is that? 120 00:10:48,730 --> 00:10:52,420 Well, this is where data gets in the way and we have the moss. 121 00:10:52,420 --> 00:10:54,219 So it's a pretty pictures of the moss. 122 00:10:54,220 --> 00:11:00,340 And this is the Time series that was collected from the 1970s to the early 2000s of the cinnabar moth interaction. 123 00:11:00,610 --> 00:11:05,379 So I've got the log numbers on the on the Y axis and that on time, every year, 124 00:11:05,380 --> 00:11:10,360 every year this person went out based in the Netherlands, just just south of Leiden. 125 00:11:10,630 --> 00:11:16,510 It's a very nice habitat. They're counting the number of regular plants and the number of moths on those plants over that time. 126 00:11:16,510 --> 00:11:21,610 So and we see that the black line is the moths and the red dotted line is the plants. 127 00:11:22,180 --> 00:11:28,120 Okay. It doesn't really show that sort of that sort of expected dynamic. 128 00:11:28,600 --> 00:11:31,989 So we've got to think why why might that not be the case? 129 00:11:31,990 --> 00:11:41,319 And we'll get onto that very shortly. But it's actually sort of like brings in thinking about other bits of of biology and maths. 130 00:11:41,320 --> 00:11:48,610 And we want to think about what we don't see that nice stable interaction, not nice stable dynamics of the moss. 131 00:11:48,610 --> 00:11:52,180 And why is that? Well, we can we can appeal. 132 00:11:52,420 --> 00:11:57,969 We've got an idea about the sorts of dynamics that we have in these these time series that we have, 133 00:11:57,970 --> 00:12:00,190 allow us to be able to do some statistical analysis. 134 00:12:01,440 --> 00:12:06,650 So I can ask basically what determines what numbers in this place in the Netherlands and how is it dependent? 135 00:12:06,660 --> 00:12:11,100 Is it dependent on the market? Is it dependent on the resources? And how do those two things interact? 136 00:12:11,430 --> 00:12:15,090 So the black line is the as I said, is the is the most numbers. 137 00:12:15,300 --> 00:12:19,260 And I'm asking basically, how do those numbers change over time from one time steps? 138 00:12:19,260 --> 00:12:20,340 And what are the drivers? 139 00:12:20,610 --> 00:12:30,240 How are the numbers in the previous time step important or two time time steps back really important in driving these sorts of population dynamics. 140 00:12:30,480 --> 00:12:34,080 And we see two things. One, we see that the models affect themselves. 141 00:12:34,440 --> 00:12:38,040 So they compete, if you like, for access to this plant in some way. 142 00:12:38,520 --> 00:12:41,190 But there's also a dependency of the plant. 143 00:12:41,190 --> 00:12:48,570 There's actually a link that that that the idea that they are specialists and they do need this plant and that plant that drives their dynamics. 144 00:12:48,810 --> 00:12:53,790 But it's more interesting than that. It isn't actually the number of plants that are around in the last year. 145 00:12:54,000 --> 00:12:58,770 It's the number of plants to around two years back that drives these sorts of dynamics of the moth. 146 00:13:00,030 --> 00:13:05,220 And we can do the same thing for the plants. We can do the time series and statistics on the plant so we can say, how do the plant numbers change? 147 00:13:05,490 --> 00:13:08,730 And it turns out, well, plants affect themselves both now. 148 00:13:09,090 --> 00:13:14,270 So the number of plants now are affected by the numbers that were around last year and the numbers are around and two years back, 149 00:13:14,520 --> 00:13:18,149 but also affected by the number of moths that are around as well. 150 00:13:18,150 --> 00:13:23,549 So we really there's a really dynamic interaction there between the plants and the moths and the moth in the plant. 151 00:13:23,550 --> 00:13:24,810 And this approximation, 152 00:13:24,810 --> 00:13:34,139 while it's really good in theory to solve doesn't it doesn't really hold when we think when we compare to data because of this inherent link. 153 00:13:34,140 --> 00:13:39,240 And if I just flip back really quickly, is this is this idea here, 154 00:13:39,240 --> 00:13:47,250 this idea of this can this this response and this link between what gets eaten and what gets and what translates into new, 155 00:13:48,000 --> 00:13:54,520 new moths in the next in the next time step. So that brings me naturally, really naturally onto this idea. 156 00:13:54,780 --> 00:13:58,950 We call that link in biology, in ecology a functional response. 157 00:13:58,960 --> 00:14:02,100 If you're a biochemist, you'll know it as a Michalis mentum function. 158 00:14:03,120 --> 00:14:09,569 It takes so many different words or phrases to explain what this what this is, 159 00:14:09,570 --> 00:14:17,880 but it's a link between two different states of the system and it's a really great way and we'll get into it surely about how you can, 160 00:14:18,150 --> 00:14:23,310 how you can approximate individual behaviour and individual performance into dynamics. 161 00:14:24,640 --> 00:14:28,240 And I want to sort of explain that is with a completely different bit of biology, 162 00:14:28,540 --> 00:14:33,579 and that is to do with the the blood stem cell system, the haematopoietic stem cell system. 163 00:14:33,580 --> 00:14:39,790 So we're really thinking about how we can how the the individual response is 164 00:14:39,790 --> 00:14:45,159 needed by we needed to make a blood system are dependent on what's around. 165 00:14:45,160 --> 00:14:48,370 It's called the demand controls it's called the demand control system. 166 00:14:48,610 --> 00:14:52,060 And your blood system is just fantastically fascinating. 167 00:14:52,450 --> 00:14:56,889 In your bone marrow, you have stem cells that are constantly making new blood cells. 168 00:14:56,890 --> 00:15:00,010 And they can make two broad types of of stem cells. 169 00:15:00,220 --> 00:15:02,980 They can make two types of blood cells. 170 00:15:03,280 --> 00:15:09,970 The lymphoid cells going down this sort of root are all the sorts of things that make your immune system, make it work. 171 00:15:10,390 --> 00:15:16,540 And the other ones are those or myeloid cells which go on to make of a whole set of red blood cells. 172 00:15:16,930 --> 00:15:22,299 And we just think about we just kind of stylise that sort of interaction into thinking about myeloid cells, 173 00:15:22,300 --> 00:15:25,660 those that make red blood cells and lymphoid cells that make immune system. 174 00:15:25,660 --> 00:15:31,150 We're not. But there's immense amount of complexity in terms of the different types of cells that can be made. 175 00:15:31,360 --> 00:15:35,380 And obviously when it goes wrong, it goes wrong and it can make people very sick. 176 00:15:35,410 --> 00:15:40,390 And we're trying to understand how we can use a mouse to actually understand the dynamics of this, 177 00:15:40,720 --> 00:15:47,920 this really incredibly diverse system and how we can use these scaling as these functional spot response scaling to understand that. 178 00:15:51,500 --> 00:15:57,489 So what we can do is that we can stylise it by going from that to a very complicated bit of biology to sound like. 179 00:15:57,490 --> 00:16:04,040 It's like saying that I could write, you know, just write out in terms of just a schematic diagram. 180 00:16:04,520 --> 00:16:11,629 So basically we were thinking about transition. So you can go, how do these how do these stem cells give rise to new stem cells? 181 00:16:11,630 --> 00:16:20,360 How do they give rise to progenitor cells? And how do those progenitor cells give rise to those myeloid or those red blood cells or white blood cells? 182 00:16:20,750 --> 00:16:26,510 And what tends that what the sort of ideas behind this that sort of so reach back to 183 00:16:26,510 --> 00:16:31,070 the 1960s we really just thought about two types of transitions that could happen. 184 00:16:31,310 --> 00:16:35,990 So this this this particular little blob represents a stem cell. 185 00:16:36,440 --> 00:16:41,150 And the things that can happen in the next time step in the next event are that I get two new stem cells, 186 00:16:41,930 --> 00:16:49,670 or I can go from a stem cell to a progenitor cell that's going to give rise to a red blood cell or a white blood cell. 187 00:16:50,060 --> 00:16:56,030 But actually, the system's a lot more complicated than that. Like, if these are the man control system, it's basically your body. 188 00:16:56,030 --> 00:17:01,220 Say, I need more. I need more red blood cells or I need more immune cells to fight off this disease. 189 00:17:01,340 --> 00:17:04,760 There has to be other things going on and we can sort of extend that out. 190 00:17:04,760 --> 00:17:09,140 So a stem cell could say there's also like an emergency response that says, actually, 191 00:17:09,260 --> 00:17:15,770 this stem cell now needs to not only it needs to sort of like die and die and make 192 00:17:15,920 --> 00:17:21,499 tuned you to new progenitor cells that then would make red or white blood cells, 193 00:17:21,500 --> 00:17:24,020 depending on how the stem cell could die, 194 00:17:24,290 --> 00:17:30,649 the progenitor cell could differentiate even further and make two new two different types of progenitor cells, two more progenitor cells. 195 00:17:30,650 --> 00:17:33,410 Because that's what happens in these in these stem cell systems, 196 00:17:33,650 --> 00:17:38,600 the progenitor cell could pass away or it could differentiate into something that's really specialised, 197 00:17:38,600 --> 00:17:41,840 making a platelet or a T cell or whatever it might be. 198 00:17:42,470 --> 00:17:51,650 So we can really think about these sorts, this sort of system in many different steps, so many, many different sort of event, event driven steps. 199 00:17:51,890 --> 00:17:56,870 And by that, I mean that in any small interval of time, only one of these is allowed to occur. 200 00:17:57,560 --> 00:18:00,560 And the way that we I'll show you what I mean by that shortly. 201 00:18:00,860 --> 00:18:07,430 But what we can do is that we can we're getting toward some more mass. We can sort of start to put some some detail on to that. 202 00:18:07,730 --> 00:18:11,540 So here's the stem cell, here's a progenitor cell and has a differentiate itself. 203 00:18:11,540 --> 00:18:14,990 So a red or white blood cell. And these are sort of intermediate cells. 204 00:18:15,200 --> 00:18:23,480 So I have some background that affects whether the stem cells are going to give birth or they're going to die or what controls their their activity. 205 00:18:24,230 --> 00:18:28,790 But the important thing to note here is this these these arrows, this one. 206 00:18:29,180 --> 00:18:34,579 So where we have this this this particular these two particular these two particular functions, 207 00:18:34,580 --> 00:18:38,000 this negative signal, that's the demand control that we're interested in. 208 00:18:38,000 --> 00:18:41,480 We're really thinking about what is it that how do we link this, 209 00:18:41,840 --> 00:18:46,310 what's needed from the body in the peripheral blood to to tell the stem cells what to do? 210 00:18:46,730 --> 00:18:47,629 Because they don't really know. 211 00:18:47,630 --> 00:18:53,540 They're just going to just say they're just going to keep making things and they need to be told what to do and they need some way of linking. 212 00:18:53,540 --> 00:18:56,090 And that's where that functional response actually comes in. 213 00:18:56,330 --> 00:19:04,069 So we've got a progression of births, if you like, of progenitor cells, births of of differentiation, differentiate themselves. 214 00:19:04,070 --> 00:19:07,100 But then when they've differentiated, we need to be able to understand, 215 00:19:07,610 --> 00:19:11,909 we need to be able to counter this idea of this negative feedback, this negative signal. 216 00:19:11,910 --> 00:19:14,960 It says, make more or make less, do something, basically. 217 00:19:15,980 --> 00:19:22,730 We can capture that again with a set of ordinary differential equations, all of that biology and are more complicated than the ones I showed earlier. 218 00:19:22,760 --> 00:19:26,750 So again, we've got this body t that's the change in stem cells through time. 219 00:19:27,170 --> 00:19:29,870 I've got deep body. T That's the progenitors through time. 220 00:19:30,080 --> 00:19:36,710 And Dee dee dee dee is the change in differentiated cells and they've got bits of biology that capture this. 221 00:19:36,920 --> 00:19:41,120 They're actually the ideas of what we expect that happens in a stem cell system. 222 00:19:41,600 --> 00:19:49,310 So this first term, this term here, this term here, depending on where you see, my point is just telling us that the stem cell has a niche. 223 00:19:49,610 --> 00:19:56,540 It has a limited amount of size that it can occupy your. Your bone marrow is limited in space, the space it occupies. 224 00:19:56,780 --> 00:20:00,230 And that means that the number of stem cells that can live in that space is going to be limited. 225 00:20:00,530 --> 00:20:07,100 And that's set by K. We call that the carrying capacity in ecology, but it's just a fixed amount of space that the stem cells can live in. 226 00:20:07,580 --> 00:20:11,540 And then then those things that can happen, they can they can give birth to new stem cells. 227 00:20:11,540 --> 00:20:17,750 All they can do. They can change these. They make progenitor cells, which is this term here. 228 00:20:17,990 --> 00:20:24,379 This is the second picture I had where a stem cell which replaces a stem cell and also makes a 229 00:20:24,380 --> 00:20:31,310 presenter cell or this one here where there's a two says there's a more emergency signal that says, 230 00:20:31,310 --> 00:20:34,760 make me two. That's that. That's the use of stem cell. 231 00:20:34,940 --> 00:20:42,970 Need to now make me two progenitor cells. Okay. And then there's and then there's lost terms that's decay term to death terms of stem cells and death, 232 00:20:42,980 --> 00:20:48,590 terms of progenitor cells and transfer of progenitor cells into into differentiate yourselves. 233 00:20:48,890 --> 00:20:54,620 But it's these two functions that are really critical, this function response at linking these dynamics, 234 00:20:54,770 --> 00:20:59,510 linking the individual performance so that to the to the population dynamics of the system. 235 00:21:00,870 --> 00:21:06,480 So if I if I use some numerical optimisation methods, I can I can iterate that model. 236 00:21:06,630 --> 00:21:11,570 So here's the number of differentiate yourself through time. The red line is the deterministic dynamic. 237 00:21:11,580 --> 00:21:18,510 So if I just run that model is just as a is an all set of a coupled order and the differential equations, I end up with this red line. 238 00:21:19,290 --> 00:21:22,619 As I said, it's a stochastic system, so it's a random system. 239 00:21:22,620 --> 00:21:30,180 So if I make this event driven, if I make those, if I make each of those transitions an event, 240 00:21:30,300 --> 00:21:34,530 I can only operate in a small, small amount, a small time stab. 241 00:21:34,980 --> 00:21:39,870 Then I get a whole raft I can. So look at this of repertoire of dynamics that I see out of this. 242 00:21:39,870 --> 00:21:41,610 What do I expect from this system, 243 00:21:42,150 --> 00:21:49,860 given that that is that it is these random to knowing whether stem cell is going to make another stem cell or a progenitor cell. 244 00:21:50,190 --> 00:21:53,070 And while this captures those actually the variability that we could see, 245 00:21:53,370 --> 00:21:59,340 it shows us is bounded in some sense that actually the system doesn't go off to infinity and it doesn't go to zero. 246 00:21:59,610 --> 00:22:06,059 There's actually some bounds. So we can run this many times and we can we can do some fancy maths and we can quantify the variability 247 00:22:06,060 --> 00:22:11,730 that we see in this in the number of differentiated cells produced by any single one stem cell. 248 00:22:12,060 --> 00:22:17,219 And this is just a spatial plot, if you like, of just a heap liposome. 249 00:22:17,220 --> 00:22:25,170 Well, here's the number of stem cells, so of around about ten, and here is the number of differentiate ourselves on average around about 40. 250 00:22:25,380 --> 00:22:31,980 But we see some spread around that that function response is, you know, not demand control is not perfect, actually. 251 00:22:31,980 --> 00:22:38,010 And I'll come on to this. Surely there's we need to understand in a little bit more detail this idea of homeostasis. 252 00:22:40,070 --> 00:22:48,320 And this idea of feedback. So how this demand feedback plays out and how it can can occur in all three Broadway's so really thinking about. 253 00:22:49,620 --> 00:22:52,499 The number of lymphoid saw a number of cells that are improved for blood. 254 00:22:52,500 --> 00:22:57,660 So we'll say the number of and the number of sort of immune cells that are around. 255 00:22:57,960 --> 00:23:02,790 And we think, well, what proportion of myeloid cells should be produced given this number of lymphoid cells? 256 00:23:03,090 --> 00:23:08,730 Well, the first one is could be completely independent. These two things in the skull could just operate independently of each other. 257 00:23:08,970 --> 00:23:13,410 So we just have the number myeloid cells. It doesn't really care how many immune cells, how many immune cells. 258 00:23:13,410 --> 00:23:17,280 So then I'm just going to make more red blood cells because that's what I set up to do. 259 00:23:17,760 --> 00:23:21,120 All that could be some relationship and proportional to need. 260 00:23:21,120 --> 00:23:26,939 So is the is that is that as there's more lymphoid cells, there's there's a need for more myeloid cells. 261 00:23:26,940 --> 00:23:30,540 So it could be non-linear, it could actually start to increase and then plateau out. 262 00:23:30,900 --> 00:23:35,830 We need to be able to understand that. We need to understand how this is so peripheral blood. 263 00:23:35,970 --> 00:23:40,080 How this peripheral blood feeds back is information to the stem cells. 264 00:23:41,500 --> 00:23:44,530 And this is the crux of it. You can write down a functional response. 265 00:23:44,540 --> 00:23:49,869 We can write down a mathematical expression that says, how do we link the the the proportion, 266 00:23:49,870 --> 00:23:54,670 the number or the proportion of cells that need to be made to what's already there? 267 00:23:55,120 --> 00:23:59,410 And we start by doing that in homeostasis, where the system is not being perturbed. 268 00:23:59,710 --> 00:24:05,830 So we think of this row H as the number of myeloid cells or the number of lymphoid 269 00:24:05,830 --> 00:24:10,510 cells that are being produced in homeostasis to maintain the body in a fixed state. 270 00:24:11,110 --> 00:24:15,099 And it's given by. So how many should I produce is in proportion to how many of that. 271 00:24:15,100 --> 00:24:18,610 This is the ratio of myeloid cells that are there to lymphoid cells that are there. 272 00:24:19,030 --> 00:24:21,790 And then there's some parameters, alpha and gamma, 273 00:24:22,030 --> 00:24:31,210 that we want to be able to understand what they look like and what values should they have that give us a homeostatic state. 274 00:24:32,490 --> 00:24:37,649 Well, if you want to know what values well, we can. So if this is homeostasis, that's a fixed state. 275 00:24:37,650 --> 00:24:41,670 We know what that is because it's homeostasis. It's not changing over time. 276 00:24:42,360 --> 00:24:46,770 So I can then sort of rearrange this expression and say, well, what values of alphas give me this? 277 00:24:47,070 --> 00:24:51,120 What values? What values of alpha should I have that give me this homeostatic state? 278 00:24:51,120 --> 00:24:56,340 And what values of gamma also give me this homeostatic state. And it turns out there's a whole family of them. 279 00:24:56,580 --> 00:24:59,280 They all look like this. There's a whole set of values. 280 00:24:59,280 --> 00:25:05,940 So here's the values of alpha and the values of Gamma at every single point on that line represents homeostasis. 281 00:25:06,750 --> 00:25:13,780 So there's a whole different value. There isn't one set of alpha, and there's not one gamma value that's going to tell me that this is homeostasis. 282 00:25:13,800 --> 00:25:22,620 It's just this whole family. Every value along this line is a point where we identify a home that will give us a homeostasis. 283 00:25:22,980 --> 00:25:24,090 Give us homeostasis. 284 00:25:24,480 --> 00:25:32,280 Well, that's pretty cool, because actually what that means is I can pick of I can pick a value of alpha and gamma and actually ask, 285 00:25:32,280 --> 00:25:34,440 what will the functional response actually look like? 286 00:25:34,830 --> 00:25:42,750 It doesn't actually look, you know, how how many so what proportion of myeloid lymphoid cells should I produce, given how many are there? 287 00:25:43,140 --> 00:25:51,240 Basically, from each of those choosing two of those choosing an alpha and an associated gamma gives me a whole set of family of of outcomes to this. 288 00:25:51,750 --> 00:25:58,710 So the functional response is it demand control? Is it that is in proportion to what's already there or is it independent of what's already there? 289 00:25:58,950 --> 00:26:05,820 Really depends on the parameter values that we have to all along that line. 290 00:26:06,060 --> 00:26:11,370 We can get a whole set of different dynamical outcomes from this, and I'm about to explain what I mean by that. 291 00:26:11,370 --> 00:26:16,919 And when we talk about predator prey interactions, but the but we have a whole family, 292 00:26:16,920 --> 00:26:21,600 a whole set of different shape functions that relate individual performance, 293 00:26:21,600 --> 00:26:29,640 the demand required so of what's in peripheral blood to what the stem cells, what those stem cells should actually do, which is pretty cool. 294 00:26:30,300 --> 00:26:34,650 And I'm going to get on to talk about predator prey interactions and I'm going to talk about the same sort of thing. 295 00:26:34,650 --> 00:26:43,889 So the same sort of thing plays out. We're really thinking about how do individual predators, moths, eating plants or whatever it might be, 296 00:26:43,890 --> 00:26:48,060 polar bears eating seals or dinosaurs eating small rats or whatever it might be. 297 00:26:48,480 --> 00:26:53,459 How do they how does that how does that behaviour of eating those things translate 298 00:26:53,460 --> 00:26:57,420 into a population dynamic outcome that affects these things through time? 299 00:26:59,110 --> 00:27:04,930 So notice that differential equations. Oh, no, no, not differential equations, but similar to the ones I introduced before. 300 00:27:05,290 --> 00:27:08,769 So I have different letters, but the same sort of biology. We're really good at this. 301 00:27:08,770 --> 00:27:12,729 In biology it does changing things with letters and not really changing much. 302 00:27:12,730 --> 00:27:14,350 But we've got predators and prey. 303 00:27:14,350 --> 00:27:23,980 So n is my numbers of prey and P's, my number of predators and damned by d d invites the change in imprinting p by d t is the change in predators. 304 00:27:24,160 --> 00:27:29,590 And I got some biology on the right hand side. So I've got a birth rate. Oh, I've got a consumption rate by predators on prey. 305 00:27:29,590 --> 00:27:37,150 I've got now I've got a conversion rate, so I'm converting dead prey into new predators, and then I've got a death rate of predators. 306 00:27:38,020 --> 00:27:43,690 So let's just scale a lot back and think, well, what sort of dynamics should I expect to have if the predators aren't there? 307 00:27:43,700 --> 00:27:46,809 So let's set pizza zero and see what happens here by six. 308 00:27:46,810 --> 00:27:51,820 So we've got DRM by D, T, R and basically what sort of dynamics do we expect to see? 309 00:27:51,830 --> 00:27:56,920 So again, we can do some maths this time. I'm not showing my workings, but I am. 310 00:27:56,920 --> 00:28:01,330 Show you a picture that shows the sorts of dynamics we get. We expect if our is positive. 311 00:28:01,660 --> 00:28:04,900 So there's positive birth as the R is greater than zero, 312 00:28:05,200 --> 00:28:11,260 then the population of rabbits in this case just will grow exponentially unbounded and they'll keep doing that right. 313 00:28:11,710 --> 00:28:14,830 So that's all that's going to us. All we expect to see in this is predator prey. 314 00:28:15,070 --> 00:28:18,340 And in this in this interaction, when the predators are not around, 315 00:28:18,340 --> 00:28:24,010 the population of rabbits, in my case will just continue to expand through time forever. 316 00:28:24,550 --> 00:28:29,230 Well, that's not going to really happen, because what's going to happen is that the predators are going to come out and start consuming rabbits. 317 00:28:29,230 --> 00:28:33,430 Basically, I'm going to think, well, actually, we really want to understand how does it as I said, 318 00:28:33,430 --> 00:28:38,889 it was really thinking about how does the individual predator eating a rabbit, 319 00:28:38,890 --> 00:28:42,970 which is a behavioural response, the predators are going to find a rabbit and catch it and eat it. 320 00:28:43,240 --> 00:28:46,690 How does that translate into population dynamics? 321 00:28:47,200 --> 00:28:56,380 Okay. So we are going to work out what this functional response looks like and do some analysis. 322 00:28:56,680 --> 00:28:59,710 So here's the opportunity. I have six bags of chocolate. 323 00:28:59,990 --> 00:29:05,709 You don't know is how many chocolates are in each bag. And will I like to do this sort of experiment? 324 00:29:05,710 --> 00:29:09,640 It's a little exercise if you do. So I need six volunteers to put your hands up. 325 00:29:09,640 --> 00:29:15,340 I will pass you a tray and I. And I'll pass your tray and a bag of chocolate, and we'll work through this. 326 00:29:17,070 --> 00:29:28,930 All right? Oh, I can guarantee the trays came from the cafeteria, and they're still warm, so they're clean. 327 00:29:30,070 --> 00:29:34,720 But I also bought clean pieces of paper as well, so feel free. 328 00:29:34,900 --> 00:29:39,160 I'm just going to put those there, remember? Just put those I remember behind our trays. 329 00:29:39,160 --> 00:29:42,970 First, take a tray. Who else can I. Can I give you a tray? 330 00:29:43,300 --> 00:29:48,410 Excellent. Oh. Can I give you a try? 331 00:29:49,950 --> 00:29:52,950 Can I give you a shake? Thanks. All right. 332 00:29:53,160 --> 00:29:56,940 Anybody on this side want to try? Excellent. Well, yeah, they're still wet. 333 00:29:57,070 --> 00:30:00,240 Well, that's disgusting. I'm sorry. You're going to get a wet tray. 334 00:30:00,630 --> 00:30:03,720 Sorry. I've got one more. I'll go over this side of the room. 335 00:30:03,960 --> 00:30:10,980 Bear with me a moment. Somebody over here is interesting enough. 336 00:30:11,850 --> 00:30:16,020 Excellent. Take your tray. It's a bit wet. Sorry. I'll be back with some chocolate in a moment. 337 00:30:19,160 --> 00:30:22,560 Right. All right. Right. 338 00:30:24,950 --> 00:30:28,700 Now, remember, I gave my trace back a piece of paper. Thank you. 339 00:30:29,150 --> 00:30:33,620 And I'm going to give you a random bag of sweets. You've probably got about 25 in there. 340 00:30:33,830 --> 00:30:39,470 You need to open them and tip onto the piece of paper. So I'm going to be a piece of paper and a bag of chocolates. 341 00:30:39,740 --> 00:30:42,800 I want I need you to do when you have a piece of paper to open the bag. 342 00:30:44,000 --> 00:30:48,500 So guess how many chocolates you have. I'll try and tell you. And just spread them out on the piece of paper. 343 00:30:49,070 --> 00:30:53,630 Probably about five or ten in there. What's your opinion? 344 00:30:54,890 --> 00:30:58,130 Somebody's got to get really lucky because one of these bags has got about 100 sweets in them. 345 00:30:58,950 --> 00:31:02,400 Oh, it's going to be a good. Oh, I'm sorry, Doctor. 346 00:31:02,420 --> 00:31:06,050 You got five. I'm sorry. Don't worry. It could get worse. 347 00:31:06,530 --> 00:31:10,760 You go back in there. Where else is my tray? 348 00:31:11,330 --> 00:31:18,050 Oh, one here. Excellent. So it's put about ten or 25 in there. 349 00:31:19,820 --> 00:31:26,660 Okay. Sorry. I have one more to do over here. 350 00:31:28,100 --> 00:31:32,070 You get the bag of Eminem's that's left in here. Well, you can't use them all. 351 00:31:32,090 --> 00:31:37,490 I'm afraid I only need you to take two out of the bag, but you can share them with the people that are around you if you like. 352 00:31:38,030 --> 00:31:42,650 So there's the chocolate, and here's the back of them. So just take two out and put them on the piece of paper. 353 00:31:43,040 --> 00:31:48,800 So if you've opened up your. So what we're going to do is we're going to do this idea of a functional response. 354 00:31:49,400 --> 00:31:56,389 And the idea is that you to you are going to be predators and you're going to find your prey on the M&Ms that you've cast out on your piece of paper. 355 00:31:56,390 --> 00:31:59,900 So take them out onto the piece of paper. If you don't know the person next to you, 356 00:31:59,900 --> 00:32:07,220 it's a great opportunity to introduce yourself and just help get them to spread them at random so you can't see where they are. 357 00:32:07,580 --> 00:32:15,050 Because what we do, we have to close our eyes. You're going to take your your your pride claw, which is actually your index finger. 358 00:32:15,710 --> 00:32:22,370 This is your index finger. I have to say this every time this is your index finger and this is what you're going to use to search for your prey, 359 00:32:22,850 --> 00:32:26,000 not your hand, your index finger. Okay. 360 00:32:26,210 --> 00:32:33,050 What are you going to do is you're going to close your eyes and you're going to tap on the board on the piece of paper when you find a mate. 361 00:32:33,320 --> 00:32:37,040 Please don't eat it just yet. Put it on the side because we need to know how many you've got. 362 00:32:37,400 --> 00:32:42,080 So if you've all got a good idea of how many you've got on the paper. Yeah, how many you've got to start with yet. 363 00:32:42,500 --> 00:32:46,800 You've got ten. Somebody's got 100. Yeah. 364 00:32:46,920 --> 00:32:50,170 Somebody got 50. Who's got 50? Stick your hand up. 365 00:32:50,170 --> 00:32:53,670 He's got 50. Somebody must have quite a lot out there. 366 00:32:54,360 --> 00:32:59,010 Yeah. Yeah. So. So you've got some idea about how many you've got out there, right? 367 00:32:59,410 --> 00:33:03,209 Okay, so we're going to do this. So what? The idea is that you're going to close your eyes, but it's not for a minute. 368 00:33:03,210 --> 00:33:07,350 You're going to see how many you find. Okay. All right. We're going to do is going to do some analysis. 369 00:33:07,350 --> 00:33:12,510 I'm going to see what how your behaviour of tapping relates to population dynamics. 370 00:33:12,900 --> 00:33:16,560 Okay. So very happy with that. Yeah. Cool. Go happy at the back. 371 00:33:17,900 --> 00:33:21,650 Yeah. Yeah. Everybody happy? I need to hear everybody's happy. Wave if you're happy. 372 00:33:22,040 --> 00:33:26,170 Yeah. Go. I'll have you go watch Ireland. 373 00:33:26,290 --> 00:33:29,600 Have you got what? Can you take me a minute? Yeah. 374 00:33:29,690 --> 00:33:33,560 Yeah. Well, I know. Really. Excellent. Okay. Everybody ready? So you going to close your eyes? 375 00:33:33,580 --> 00:33:37,060 Index fingers, people. Hold on. Let me see an index finger. 376 00:33:37,570 --> 00:33:40,650 Index fingers. Index fingers. Thanks. 377 00:33:40,780 --> 00:33:44,370 Okay. Close your eyes. Ready? We're going to talk for a minute. 378 00:33:44,380 --> 00:34:28,990 Go. How we do. 379 00:34:34,300 --> 00:34:41,340 10 seconds. Excellent. All three. 380 00:34:43,260 --> 00:34:46,630 What? Stop. Okay, please stop. 381 00:34:47,050 --> 00:34:50,110 Hopefully the person who found two managed to find them all. 382 00:34:50,800 --> 00:34:56,420 Yeah. Excellent. That's a good start. Okay, so we're going to do the analysis now you found. 383 00:34:56,440 --> 00:35:00,010 So I just need you to count up how many you found. So somebody had five. 384 00:35:00,520 --> 00:35:04,570 How many did you get? Well done. You're a good predator. Excellent. 385 00:35:04,660 --> 00:35:08,510 Top of the chain. Excellent. Somebody had ten on the board. 386 00:35:08,530 --> 00:35:12,250 How many did you find? Three. You're not so good. 387 00:35:12,840 --> 00:35:16,260 At least you got them to eat for the rest of their lecture, even if I'm boring. So that's quite good. 388 00:35:16,530 --> 00:35:20,650 Somebody had a 25 on the board. How did you two get? 389 00:35:21,550 --> 00:35:25,570 22. Did you use your index finger or a whole hand? Excellent. 390 00:35:25,750 --> 00:35:29,290 Good one. Always good to know. 22. All right. 391 00:35:29,540 --> 00:35:33,599 Somebody had 50. 3838. 392 00:35:33,600 --> 00:35:38,010 Wow. My God. This is going to be interesting. All right. 393 00:35:38,070 --> 00:35:42,060 So and so. The person in the back. 42. 42. 394 00:35:42,090 --> 00:35:46,110 Excellent. All right. Let's see how this works. 395 00:35:47,400 --> 00:35:52,550 So what we're going to do. I am going to. Quickly make sure I've done this right. 396 00:35:53,660 --> 00:35:59,690 Oh, there we go. Always an issue when it doesn't like the numbers, but, hey, he's got a bit cranky. 397 00:36:01,010 --> 00:36:07,220 Can I go to work? Okay, it's not gonna work. So, what, we. Okay, I'm just going to fudge the numbers, but you'll see just a little bit. 398 00:36:07,250 --> 00:36:13,010 Just to make it look. Let's see if we can make this. Just to give you an idea of what happens. 399 00:36:14,290 --> 00:36:18,520 So the two things that two things that go, the two types of responses that go on. 400 00:36:18,530 --> 00:36:23,890 So I have a picture somewhat here. So you guys were really great. 401 00:36:24,010 --> 00:36:27,910 Without a shadow of a doubt, my analysis doesn't take into account the fact that you could be linear. 402 00:36:28,090 --> 00:36:34,540 The fact is that what we have on this graph sorry, the font is not great on a but on the x axis I have the number of M&M that are 403 00:36:34,540 --> 00:36:38,980 on the board and on the y axis I have the number that were caught in a minute. 404 00:36:39,310 --> 00:36:44,080 And what you guys have done is managed to find whether you have 100 on the board. 405 00:36:44,080 --> 00:36:48,159 You found half of them when you found when you have 50 on the board, you got quite a lot. 406 00:36:48,160 --> 00:36:51,250 I know the idea is that that response is linear. 407 00:36:51,250 --> 00:36:56,110 It will go the I've I've fudged the points because we're trying to fit a curve through this. 408 00:36:56,290 --> 00:37:03,850 We you would have a nice linear relationship there of the number court and the number the number on the board and the number that you caught. 409 00:37:04,420 --> 00:37:09,720 What tends to happen when we do this and we replicate and then obviously I can only do this with six of you if you all 410 00:37:09,730 --> 00:37:14,620 were able to do this and we were in a little bit more of a controlled environment when we were doing this experiment. 411 00:37:14,620 --> 00:37:20,259 What would happen is that over time, even when you had how many did you find when you had 100 on the board? 412 00:37:20,260 --> 00:37:23,350 40 to 42. Right. So you don't find them. 413 00:37:23,350 --> 00:37:29,680 All right. You find about half of them. And the really important thing there is that actually what we're doing is we're linking the behaviour, 414 00:37:29,890 --> 00:37:36,760 this idea of, of, of, of foraging for the prey to the population dynamics. 415 00:37:36,760 --> 00:37:41,140 And what we expect to see is that as you increase, as you increase on the X axis, 416 00:37:41,440 --> 00:37:46,419 you can't actually get many more than about 40 out of 30 or something out there. 417 00:37:46,420 --> 00:37:50,499 So it doesn't there's a finite amount of time that you have to actually be able 418 00:37:50,500 --> 00:37:55,270 to take these Eminem's off the board and put them on the side or as a predator, 419 00:37:55,600 --> 00:37:59,290 kill your zebra and convert that zebra into a new lion. 420 00:37:59,530 --> 00:38:04,599 That takes time. You have to eat the thing. You have to digest it. You have to you have to give birth to new lions. 421 00:38:04,600 --> 00:38:08,350 That's a has a time dependent response and it has this particular shape. 422 00:38:08,860 --> 00:38:17,200 Okay. So as the number increase, as I have numbers up on this axis at the moment against the numbers on the on on the on the board. 423 00:38:18,040 --> 00:38:21,580 But if I think instead of having numbers, if I have the proportion, 424 00:38:21,790 --> 00:38:25,780 what happens with this graph is that actually down at the low end, most things get called. 425 00:38:26,230 --> 00:38:32,740 You know, that might be counterintuitive, but actually if there's a small number of prey, most of them are going to get captured. 426 00:38:33,070 --> 00:38:37,070 But when there's a large number of prey. They don't all get caught. 427 00:38:37,080 --> 00:38:40,410 And as the number as the number increases, they continue to. 428 00:38:40,740 --> 00:38:44,430 They continue to they continues to go down. 429 00:38:44,640 --> 00:38:51,060 So we have this negative relationship. So the proportion eaten against the number on the board goes down. 430 00:38:51,270 --> 00:38:59,310 And that basically means out here, when we have lots of prey, then we expect to see many of those prey. 431 00:39:00,550 --> 00:39:06,490 Giving are escaping predation and are able then to do whatever they do give birth. 432 00:39:07,000 --> 00:39:09,580 So we expect to see that actually when there's a large number of prey, 433 00:39:09,850 --> 00:39:15,759 this when there's when the small number of prey are being caught, lots of them will be able to increase in the absence of predation. 434 00:39:15,760 --> 00:39:18,850 As we talk and as we showed earlier, they all increase exponentially. 435 00:39:19,690 --> 00:39:24,040 So what is that? What does that what is it how does that translate to my moth dynamics? 436 00:39:26,170 --> 00:39:31,329 So I have this sort of idea, I have this sort of relationship that actually when there's when we have this dynamic, 437 00:39:31,330 --> 00:39:38,049 when they have this functional response between behaviour and dynamics that give rise to this idea that when there's lots of them around, 438 00:39:38,050 --> 00:39:41,050 very few, proportionately, very few of them are going to get eaten. 439 00:39:41,350 --> 00:39:45,070 Lots of them will escape. Predation means that they can grow exponentially. 440 00:39:45,310 --> 00:39:49,879 And then what happens is that the predator catch up is a numerical game. So there's more prey around. 441 00:39:49,880 --> 00:39:52,900 There's going to be eventually more predators around. And that's going to turn over. 442 00:39:53,110 --> 00:39:55,240 And we get these predator prey type cycles. 443 00:39:55,480 --> 00:40:01,390 And that's what we see in this system, basically, that we see the again, this is the the moth numbers through time. 444 00:40:01,480 --> 00:40:05,590 The black line is the most the morphs, the red dotted line is the plants. 445 00:40:05,590 --> 00:40:10,059 And we see this cyclic dynamic playing out. And that's because of the relationship. 446 00:40:10,060 --> 00:40:15,010 The link between the scaling between individual performance, 447 00:40:15,310 --> 00:40:20,710 how a predator or a moth in this case performs, and how that translates into the population dynamics. 448 00:40:21,340 --> 00:40:25,120 And that's pretty cool. It's a pretty cool way of which linking, you know, different, 449 00:40:25,120 --> 00:40:30,160 different aspects of behaviour operates on a different temporal scale to population dynamics. 450 00:40:30,580 --> 00:40:34,930 And when we can take that, we can translate it into these sorts of population dynamics. 451 00:40:36,950 --> 00:40:43,030 So to finish, I want to sort of like talk about this sort of like the third way that we can sort of think about mass gatherings. 452 00:40:43,040 --> 00:40:45,650 And it's a bit mathematical, but it's about approximations, 453 00:40:45,860 --> 00:40:50,179 how we can take approximate how we can approximate dynamics to operate on a different time scale. 454 00:40:50,180 --> 00:40:54,740 It's like the fast, slow dynamics, but we can do it in a lot better and a little bit more rigorous way. 455 00:40:55,310 --> 00:41:03,380 And I want to also motivate this by a really useful and so really cool way in which we approach this sort of analysis. 456 00:41:03,650 --> 00:41:07,910 We are interested and a group of colleagues in Oxford and elsewhere. 457 00:41:08,150 --> 00:41:13,160 We're interested in being able to understand the mood dynamics associated with patients with bipolar disorder. 458 00:41:13,580 --> 00:41:20,570 So bipolar are very, very common mental illness that occurs in late teens, early twenties, 459 00:41:20,720 --> 00:41:25,730 and is so a lifelong chronic condition where you get episodes of mania and depression. 460 00:41:26,030 --> 00:41:28,969 But in between, those episodes of mania and depression are really, 461 00:41:28,970 --> 00:41:35,690 really rare compared to the mood fluctuations that go on on a day to day or a week to week basis. 462 00:41:36,050 --> 00:41:41,810 And what we can use there is a set of standardised psychological scoring system sets, statistic sets, 463 00:41:42,050 --> 00:41:48,440 standardised psychological scoring metrics that can be used to quantify mood, and they're called quints. 464 00:41:48,710 --> 00:41:55,310 So it's a quick inventory of depressed symptom, symptom or symptoms, and we can use that as a self-reporting system. 465 00:41:55,310 --> 00:42:05,120 People use their cell phones or the computer to answer 16 questions that give a give an answer to, and we can create an aggregated score of that. 466 00:42:05,420 --> 00:42:12,830 So this is what this is. That's what these are the scores. Those are the mood scores that we get out of patients over a period of time. 467 00:42:13,220 --> 00:42:17,299 And we can do that. We can do that on very different temporal scales. 468 00:42:17,300 --> 00:42:23,090 We can do that on a daily basis. We can do it on a weekly basis. We can do it before and after intervention analysis. 469 00:42:23,090 --> 00:42:30,470 This is just a sort of cohort experiment that we had for 14, 15 patients or so where we monitor that mood, 470 00:42:30,620 --> 00:42:38,510 which is the, the, the solid black line over a 28 day period before and after a treatment intervention. 471 00:42:38,720 --> 00:42:44,480 My colleagues I work with with this are a clinical psychologist and they use behavioural interventions. 472 00:42:44,480 --> 00:42:48,860 So cognitive behavioural therapy to treat people and help people with bipolar disorder. 473 00:42:49,250 --> 00:42:55,399 What we can do is we can ask the question is does the mood fluctuations change before or after treatment? 474 00:42:55,400 --> 00:43:00,080 So we can quantify it before they go into treatment for four weeks and we can come out. 475 00:43:00,080 --> 00:43:03,389 They come out the other side and we can characterise their mood dynamics. 476 00:43:03,390 --> 00:43:04,100 So this is what we see. 477 00:43:04,100 --> 00:43:11,659 We're able to sort of like characterise their these mood profiles and just collect them in a, in a temporal time series sequence. 478 00:43:11,660 --> 00:43:17,660 So again, it's just, it's just population dynamics, it's just population ecology, the sort of stuff I grew up doing. 479 00:43:17,840 --> 00:43:20,720 So with a very clinical relevance here. 480 00:43:21,380 --> 00:43:28,790 So what we can do is that we can sort of like pick that apart a little bit and we can ask basically how can we understand mood dynamics? 481 00:43:28,790 --> 00:43:36,470 How can we get to the mass mood, mass we call it, how can we unpick the dynamic, those things that drive mood fluctuations? 482 00:43:37,160 --> 00:43:40,819 And we've done a bunch of things. We've used a whole set of different mathematical methods. 483 00:43:40,820 --> 00:43:45,760 And I'm just going to so I'll talk to you about this one and how we can then link this to some scaling dynamics, 484 00:43:45,950 --> 00:43:49,070 how we scale between different things that may be going on. 485 00:43:49,580 --> 00:43:51,350 So again, another just so a differential equation, 486 00:43:51,350 --> 00:43:57,169 we're going to split mood up into sort of like take them, we'll take the words, we've got change in mood. 487 00:43:57,170 --> 00:44:02,390 So from one time step to the next, very small time step, I'm not going to be explicit about what that is at the moment. 488 00:44:02,660 --> 00:44:08,600 And then we can split up and we can use a total derivative approach. We can split it up into different component parts, essentially. 489 00:44:08,990 --> 00:44:12,080 So I've got some sort of like partial derivative mood respect to time. 490 00:44:12,080 --> 00:44:18,469 So that might just be baseline mood. What happens, you know, this just happens to your mood and just baseline and then we can. 491 00:44:18,470 --> 00:44:25,760 So spitting out further into something to do with fluctuations some things driving this mood fluctuations in our in our heads. 492 00:44:25,760 --> 00:44:28,460 And we've got to really think about how we understand that. 493 00:44:28,910 --> 00:44:35,690 And we can so split that we can sort of think of this the X by D, T and D, Z by T as oscillator, something that's driving that. 494 00:44:35,690 --> 00:44:42,380 So they may be innervate, inhibitory and accumulating oscillators that drive dynamics. 495 00:44:42,680 --> 00:44:47,719 And then there's all these sorts of things. The partial change in mood with respect to X is just a slope. 496 00:44:47,720 --> 00:44:54,140 It's just a regression on how mood changes with respect to the size of the the oscillator that we're 497 00:44:54,290 --> 00:45:01,400 although the the inhibitor or excitatory effect that we're trying to measure so we can write out this. 498 00:45:01,670 --> 00:45:07,170 So this differential equation in terms of the change in mood is just baseline mood that's just going to be flat. 499 00:45:07,170 --> 00:45:12,350 So some constant that's going to change over that's going to that's not going to change over time. 500 00:45:12,620 --> 00:45:20,509 And then again, these things are going to be so there's going to be fluctuations to do this oscillator and this oscillator with some with some 501 00:45:20,510 --> 00:45:26,600 magnitude associated with the parameters in front of that and what we wanted to be able to do and we can do that statistically. 502 00:45:26,600 --> 00:45:32,480 We've done that statistically. And that was what was on the previous slide is what we can try and fit these sorts of models to the Time series. 503 00:45:34,060 --> 00:45:38,210 But I want to get into a little bit more about this. And there's a lot of words and there's a lot of detail on this slide. 504 00:45:38,530 --> 00:45:47,469 And what I want to just explain is that we can we can write that this differential equation or this differential equation as a as an oscillator. 505 00:45:47,470 --> 00:45:55,770 So if thinking about things that inhibit and things that accumulate sort of like this, these dynamical fluctuations and that really cool way, 506 00:45:55,780 --> 00:46:01,840 one of the really cool ways to be able to capture that and phenomenological very broadly without too much mechanistic detail, 507 00:46:02,080 --> 00:46:07,510 is with what we call a relaxation oscillator. And a relaxation oscillator is a really cool thing. 508 00:46:07,510 --> 00:46:12,100 It's a to say it's going to oscillate, it's going to go up and down through time and it's relaxation because actually I'm going 509 00:46:12,100 --> 00:46:16,420 to spend a long time in one state and I'm going to relax very quickly to another state. 510 00:46:16,720 --> 00:46:22,480 So it's just like a friend to price cycle might be. One way to think of it is I'm going to stay in a high state and I'm going to stay there. 511 00:46:22,480 --> 00:46:26,800 And then I go to relaxed very quickly to a low state, and then I go relax out of that state to a high state again. 512 00:46:27,250 --> 00:46:35,770 And we can capture that. We can capture that phenomena logically, very broadly with this this this ordinary differential equation. 513 00:46:35,770 --> 00:46:37,450 So we can couple two things together. 514 00:46:37,720 --> 00:46:44,140 We can put some complicated nonlinear function in there, and then we can capture this idea of a relaxation oscillator. 515 00:46:44,530 --> 00:46:49,419 Well, for bipolar disorder, that's pretty cool, because then we can think about that as I see you're in a state, 516 00:46:49,420 --> 00:46:53,470 maybe you're in a high mood state and you can relax to a baseline state. 517 00:46:53,710 --> 00:46:57,370 You may be in a low mood state and you can relax to a sort of baseline state. 518 00:46:57,370 --> 00:47:01,480 So we are capturing that idea. One idea that might be one idea that we're trying to capture. 519 00:47:01,790 --> 00:47:10,390 We may also be trying to capture this idea that there's inhibition and excitatory processes going on within our heads to sort of drive the dynamics. 520 00:47:10,750 --> 00:47:14,170 We can do other things. So it's a little bit more complicated stuff on this slide. 521 00:47:14,170 --> 00:47:18,650 There's some stuff at the end. So this stuff here and this stuff here is capturing the idea. 522 00:47:18,680 --> 00:47:23,559 We can make these things stochastic so we can kick the system with a little bit of randomness each time. 523 00:47:23,560 --> 00:47:31,690 So that just to add some just to add some additional fluctuations in that that may not be captured very broadly by this by this oscillator. 524 00:47:32,020 --> 00:47:35,110 And the other cool thing we can do is that we can couple these things together. 525 00:47:35,350 --> 00:47:39,250 We can say, actually, what's happening to one isn't independent of what is happening in the other. 526 00:47:39,490 --> 00:47:44,139 So we can really make this these sources, this sort of scaling. But when we can, as I said, 527 00:47:44,140 --> 00:47:51,400 we can fit that to the Time series so we can introduce noise and we can do or we can look at all of these couplings and we can see we 528 00:47:51,400 --> 00:48:00,250 can challenge these models against the data and look at what the what the explanations might be that best describe individual patients. 529 00:48:00,250 --> 00:48:03,879 So you as an individual patient with bipolar you need to oscillators. 530 00:48:03,880 --> 00:48:07,030 Do they have to be couple? Do they have to be noisy? And it really is. 531 00:48:07,030 --> 00:48:10,720 And it really helps us understand the temporal dynamics of bipolar. 532 00:48:12,000 --> 00:48:15,710 So it's just what we do. We consult those three patients just picked at random. 533 00:48:15,930 --> 00:48:19,620 And here's the oscillators that are all oscillating. 534 00:48:19,620 --> 00:48:25,919 They don't have to oscillate. The parameter values, the parameters in the in that oscillator could actually give rise to stable dynamics. 535 00:48:25,920 --> 00:48:29,790 It's just what values they choose choose to take on when we fit them to the Time series. 536 00:48:30,120 --> 00:48:34,740 But each of them, as you can see, everybody each of these three patients has an idiosyncratic. 537 00:48:35,590 --> 00:48:39,340 So a light oscillator system. Sometimes they have to be coupled. 538 00:48:39,340 --> 00:48:42,790 Sometimes they don't. Sometimes they're not. Sometimes they have to be noisy. Sometimes they don't. 539 00:48:43,060 --> 00:48:49,180 And we can fit those to the times, as you can see how good a fit they are, which is the purple dots in this and this in this case, 540 00:48:49,450 --> 00:48:55,059 it's really good technical challenge because actually bipolar patients are pretty bad reporting every time. 541 00:48:55,060 --> 00:48:58,090 So there's lots of missing values in this and we have to be able to deal with those statistically. 542 00:48:58,390 --> 00:49:03,280 And that's all pretty cool, too. Well, we can do that. That's pretty nice. But that's not what I want to get to. 543 00:49:03,640 --> 00:49:09,880 I want to take you to where we are with all of this, because I think it's really cool because what we're trying to do is really understand. 544 00:49:10,240 --> 00:49:16,840 So I've explained this. This is my total derivative. I've got the baseline mood, I've got some parameters associated with these fluctuations, 545 00:49:16,840 --> 00:49:25,870 but we're really trying to do is to step down into heads and think about how the neuronal firing patterns drive mood dynamics. 546 00:49:26,230 --> 00:49:27,730 Well, it's a huge ask, right? 547 00:49:27,730 --> 00:49:35,680 We're actually things no neurone firing dynamics are operating on such a fast time scale compared to how fast your mood changes. 548 00:49:36,160 --> 00:49:40,360 And I like my mood, but my moves, well, I don't know, up and down at the moment, cause I'm standing here talking. 549 00:49:40,360 --> 00:49:45,639 But, you know, it changes, you know, not as fast as a my neurones are going like, oh my God, I've got to keep talking. 550 00:49:45,640 --> 00:49:49,270 They're really firing off in my head to of hopefully make me lucid. 551 00:49:49,510 --> 00:49:57,339 But what we really want to do is to link these sort of these sorts of neurone firing patterns to the to these to this population dynamic, 552 00:49:57,340 --> 00:50:02,709 this, this dynamic of mood change over time. And these are operating on different time scales. 553 00:50:02,710 --> 00:50:07,060 And the way that we're going, the way we're thinking about approaching that is actually approximating these functions. 554 00:50:07,060 --> 00:50:14,440 So these are the these two differential equations are describing the inhibitory and excitatory processes that go on in your own firing patterns. 555 00:50:14,680 --> 00:50:20,290 And we can aggregate those together and we can really think about how we how we can describe we can just take those, 556 00:50:20,500 --> 00:50:28,090 those two differential equations, these two here, and really get into the neurobiology and the theoretical neurobiology and neurobiology of that. 557 00:50:28,450 --> 00:50:31,870 Well, we want to take you up a scale, but these are operating on different time scales. 558 00:50:31,870 --> 00:50:35,200 And the way that we can do that is to approximate these things with a Taylor series. 559 00:50:35,590 --> 00:50:40,510 And a Taylor series is just a way of writing out a function in an infinite sum, 560 00:50:40,870 --> 00:50:43,840 and we can write it out in terms of what at a particular point in time. 561 00:50:44,380 --> 00:50:47,500 And we can take that function and we can just say, Well, what is it, what is it? 562 00:50:47,530 --> 00:50:51,879 What is its linear component and what is its quadratic component? 563 00:50:51,880 --> 00:50:58,420 What is the cubic component? What is its quartette component? And we can just keep adding on it forever and we can get an understanding. 564 00:50:58,420 --> 00:51:07,750 We can approximate these neurone dynamics at a particular point in the mood dynamics with this Taylor expansion, and that holds out great possibility. 565 00:51:08,110 --> 00:51:13,990 It's where we are. We're at this step, and this is where I'll take you to where we are in this sort of problem. 566 00:51:14,200 --> 00:51:15,580 I don't have any answers to that yet, 567 00:51:15,880 --> 00:51:23,590 but we think it's a really cool approach to be able to approximate the neurone dynamics in terms of using these these sorts of Taylor expansions. 568 00:51:25,190 --> 00:51:30,530 So let me finish up with another sort of implication about scaling a different one, completely different one. 569 00:51:30,800 --> 00:51:37,310 Just another little game we'll play very shortly. What I should do is sort of like thinking about bio control people in my group. 570 00:51:37,430 --> 00:51:44,240 I guess a sat around here are interested in thinking about how we control pests, 571 00:51:44,570 --> 00:51:49,790 insect pests, vectors that spread disease, how we can think about the biology, 572 00:51:50,090 --> 00:51:51,559 biological control of doing that, 573 00:51:51,560 --> 00:51:59,420 and how maths helps us understand that there's a very old method in insect control called the sterile insect technique. 574 00:51:59,750 --> 00:52:06,140 And the idea is that you would mass these insects that lower radiation, make them sterile, 575 00:52:06,230 --> 00:52:09,980 release them out into the environment, and when they make, they make no viable offspring. 576 00:52:10,340 --> 00:52:14,450 So they don't so they don't hatch or the insects die or whatever it might be. 577 00:52:14,450 --> 00:52:22,880 So there's the idea being that you're aiming to sort of like to send the population into decline because the meetings are not successful. 578 00:52:23,450 --> 00:52:28,670 Well, there are many new twists on that. And one of the latest ones is being able to use genetics to do that. 579 00:52:28,970 --> 00:52:34,550 So you can genetically modify your favourite agricultural pest or your mosquito spreading disease. 580 00:52:34,880 --> 00:52:38,090 You can change its genes so that when it goes and you must rear them again, 581 00:52:38,300 --> 00:52:43,610 throw them out when they mate with wild types, they don't make any viable offspring. 582 00:52:43,610 --> 00:52:47,450 Those those larvae may die or the caterpillars may die or the eggs may die. 583 00:52:47,840 --> 00:52:53,600 So the idea being that you're trying to suppress the population, we can do it both conventionally with radiation or with genetics. 584 00:52:53,600 --> 00:52:56,750 So the idea is essentially to suppress the population. 585 00:52:57,950 --> 00:53:03,680 There's a whole set of cool maths behind all of that, and it's to do with what we think about as optimal control. 586 00:53:04,160 --> 00:53:10,760 We can do optimisation, we can do static optimisation, we can say, actually, we want to just know what this is for some fixed values, 587 00:53:10,760 --> 00:53:15,870 but actually this is a dynamic problem and somebody said You should shock people. 588 00:53:15,870 --> 00:53:18,169 Mike So here's the shocking bit of this still. 589 00:53:18,170 --> 00:53:24,620 So this is a bit of an old poster, but actually still something like 2000 kids die every day from malaria. 590 00:53:24,830 --> 00:53:28,580 Since I've been speaking over the last hour, probably 50 kids have died from malaria. 591 00:53:28,940 --> 00:53:31,160 What we have is a completely preventable disease. 592 00:53:31,400 --> 00:53:37,219 And using these sorts of methods, these bio control methods, we have an opportunity to be able to do that on our mats. 593 00:53:37,220 --> 00:53:40,820 Really helps us think about that because actually we've got a dynamic. 594 00:53:40,820 --> 00:53:49,550 So this equation at the top DV by d t is the change in the vector numbers, the change in the number of mosquitoes over time. 595 00:53:49,910 --> 00:53:53,630 And it's something to do. There's some bursts of mosquitoes and some deaths of mosquitoes. 596 00:53:54,230 --> 00:53:59,150 And we can fact this thing in brackets is the function that says, I'm going to do this style insect release. 597 00:53:59,150 --> 00:54:05,690 I'm either going to do it genetically or I'm going to do it conventionally, and I'm going to release a number of modified mosquitoes. 598 00:54:05,690 --> 00:54:11,540 I will if i0vtv over v t, it's just going to be is going to be one. 599 00:54:11,540 --> 00:54:16,700 So it's going to cancel out. But it's actually as I increase the number of these star releases, 600 00:54:16,970 --> 00:54:22,580 this term is going to become more important and this term is going to end in a whole term on the first term, 601 00:54:22,580 --> 00:54:24,380 on the right hand side is going to get smaller. 602 00:54:24,650 --> 00:54:31,760 We're aiming to reduce the number of births, but it's not not all that what's happening in this system when we're thinking about malaria, 603 00:54:32,030 --> 00:54:35,120 that we've got people being infected, we've got infected vector. 604 00:54:35,130 --> 00:54:42,800 So we've got a dynamic situation there. We've got dynamics operating in terms of the changes in the vector numbers, being born and being controlled. 605 00:54:43,040 --> 00:54:48,619 What we're trying to do, we're trying to stop people getting sick. So actually we can think about this as an optimal control problem. 606 00:54:48,620 --> 00:54:55,490 This is a dynamic control problem in that we're thinking about actually what we want to do is we want to control this problem over time, 607 00:54:55,640 --> 00:54:59,240 a fixed period of time, we're going to discount it. That's an economics term. 608 00:54:59,600 --> 00:55:02,090 It's a way of scaling thinking in the future. 609 00:55:02,390 --> 00:55:06,620 And we're going to think about what we want to do is we want to minimise this function in terms of the number of people 610 00:55:06,620 --> 00:55:13,490 that are getting sick and the number of the so the number of these sterile insects that we have to put out there. 611 00:55:13,880 --> 00:55:19,010 So also optimisation, we need to this has to be bigger than zero, but we don't want it to be too bigger than zero because that's going to cost us. 612 00:55:19,010 --> 00:55:22,880 It's going to be really costly to do that economically. It's going to be costly to do that. 613 00:55:23,390 --> 00:55:27,650 And this is where we're able to scale both biology and economics. 614 00:55:28,130 --> 00:55:37,580 And what I'd like to do you just to finish up is just to sit on my group, on my on our on our group website. 615 00:55:38,850 --> 00:55:44,690 It's just going to work. We have a game, we can play this game and we'll just play this game because it's really it's really instructions. 616 00:55:44,690 --> 00:55:49,579 It's an ultimate optimal control game and it's so set out without any of the math detail. 617 00:55:49,580 --> 00:55:53,770 It's got lots of the biology in there. So one of the first things you need to do when you're thinking about this, 618 00:55:54,410 --> 00:56:00,980 there's this economics of biocontrol is to know the problem, know what the size of the problem is that you're trying to control. 619 00:56:01,370 --> 00:56:07,639 And the way, way that we do that is we have to estimate, well, the big what the insect, the pest population of the vector population is. 620 00:56:07,640 --> 00:56:12,140 How big is it? Well, ecologists do that with a method called the mark release recapture method. 621 00:56:12,140 --> 00:56:15,980 So you mock a bunch of insects, you throw them out, you wait a bit time, 622 00:56:15,980 --> 00:56:18,920 you go back and you collect a bunch and you work out the proportion that were marked, 623 00:56:18,920 --> 00:56:26,239 which is a mouse, and it tells you what you can use as some wacky formula and it tells you the number of of of mosquitoes. 624 00:56:26,240 --> 00:56:30,110 Oh, that was the wrong one, but. Yes. 625 00:56:30,120 --> 00:56:34,360 Go back. Did you do the record? 626 00:56:34,450 --> 00:56:36,819 No, I did not. Okay. So are you guys not going to do it? 627 00:56:36,820 --> 00:56:41,500 Because we with kids, we get them little kids, we get them to run around and they can find mosquitoes and can do all of this. 628 00:56:41,560 --> 00:56:46,930 So if I pick a number of how many mosquitoes we want in the population, there's some stuff about the biology. 629 00:56:47,500 --> 00:56:53,630 Well, this is where it becomes important. We can now try to optimise this problem and trying to make it as cost effective as possible. 630 00:56:53,660 --> 00:56:57,920 We're trying to scale the economics and the biology to make this as cost effective as possible. 631 00:56:58,160 --> 00:57:00,230 And there are two things. The two things you can control. 632 00:57:00,470 --> 00:57:06,500 Firstly, that I how many sterile mosquitoes, how many sterile insects am I going to put out there? 633 00:57:06,830 --> 00:57:12,890 And then I got to think, how long can I run my program for? So the top one is the ecology, and the bottom one is the economics. 634 00:57:13,200 --> 00:57:18,980 Okay, so on the top one, I can choose any number between one and ten. 635 00:57:20,150 --> 00:57:23,690 Excuse me. A number between one and ten to ask a good number. 636 00:57:24,300 --> 00:57:28,520 Okay, so that means I've got to pay for every wildtype mosquito I'm going to put out there. 637 00:57:28,910 --> 00:57:32,480 I'm going to release two style insects. Okay. 638 00:57:32,720 --> 00:57:40,140 How long do you wanna run your program for? You can have two, five or ten. Well, five was the first number I heard. 639 00:57:40,170 --> 00:57:43,590 That's a good number. Okay, so this we'll see what happens. 640 00:57:44,910 --> 00:57:46,049 So we're going at two graphs. 641 00:57:46,050 --> 00:57:51,120 One of the top one, we're going to do the ecology of the mosquito, the dynamics, the population dynamics of the mosquito. 642 00:57:51,420 --> 00:57:55,230 On the bottom one, we're going to see how many people you managed to save with your particular program. 643 00:57:55,590 --> 00:57:58,980 Okay. And we're going to look for a dollar figure. 644 00:57:59,100 --> 00:58:03,990 And we want that to be as small as possible. The bigger is, the worse it's going to be, essentially. 645 00:58:04,440 --> 00:58:10,110 So I'm going to start on the other thing of ecology is that I need to know when in the season I'm going to start my control. 646 00:58:10,410 --> 00:58:17,729 I'm just going to pick one of these. I'm going to see two lines, basically. And the black line on the top is what I did in the absence of control. 647 00:58:17,730 --> 00:58:24,090 And the green line is what I do in the presence of control. And you can see that actually, I can reduce my. 648 00:58:24,330 --> 00:58:29,130 So I'm doing a 2 to 1 release ratio. So two sterile mosquitoes to every wild type. 649 00:58:29,580 --> 00:58:36,150 And on the bottom, I have the number of sick people in red that in the absence of control and in the presence of control in blue. 650 00:58:36,660 --> 00:58:41,130 And it goes up and down because we're talking about mosquitoes. And they like it wet and dry or they like it. 651 00:58:41,340 --> 00:58:48,180 They don't like it dry. So there's some seasonal dynamics that are essentially what I call things about this is actually when I got to the numbers. 652 00:58:48,180 --> 00:58:53,040 Yeah, well let's talk about the dynamics is not on the top. I only suppressed my population a little bit. 653 00:58:53,050 --> 00:58:57,330 There's 2 to 1 release ratio. It doesn't actually squash the population down very far at all. 654 00:58:57,760 --> 00:59:03,650 Yeah, it's still it's only been suppressed a little bit. But actually I can reduce the disease dynamics by about half the number. 655 00:59:03,750 --> 00:59:07,140 The average number between the red line and the blue line is gone down by half. 656 00:59:07,590 --> 00:59:09,510 So that's pretty cool, right? Actually, I do know. 657 00:59:09,690 --> 00:59:15,300 So one of the things is I don't actually need to eradicate my mosquitoes from the system to start to have an effect on disease control. 658 00:59:15,780 --> 00:59:18,890 If I choose a bigger number, it will be more costly to do that. 659 00:59:18,900 --> 00:59:25,050 And actually, I could get the same sort of phenomena. And as we know, this is a well-known this is a well known fact in epidemiology, 660 00:59:25,260 --> 00:59:33,089 where if you suppress those agricultural pests or those disease vectors be a lower threshold, then, you know, you don't have to eradicate them. 661 00:59:33,090 --> 00:59:38,069 You just have to push them down below that threshold. Then you can have a big effect on the disease dynamics. 662 00:59:38,070 --> 00:59:42,750 We stop our control. After five years, we run out of cash. So that stops after five years. 663 00:59:42,990 --> 00:59:48,780 And actually then because of reinvention, there's really good documented evidence of that happening throughout the world. 664 00:59:49,080 --> 00:59:52,560 So actually we see the disease rebounding to pre control levels. 665 00:59:54,180 --> 00:59:59,120 So what happens then? The most important thing is what this dollar figure is for the better. 666 00:59:59,370 --> 01:00:02,790 The bottom. How many cases did we avert? How many cases do we avoid? 667 01:00:03,000 --> 01:00:05,440 And how much did it cost? How much was the control enough? 668 01:00:05,670 --> 01:00:11,190 What was the optimal value that we can get out of that turns out to 52 is not bad, but we can be better. 669 01:00:11,400 --> 01:00:18,330 I had a seven year old girl do this one time. She spent 45 minutes playing on this, this, this, this game, and she came out with 229. 670 01:00:18,600 --> 01:00:22,640 I've never got down to 229. So if anybody plays this game, it gets below to 29. 671 01:00:22,830 --> 01:00:28,860 I want to know because that's really is really important thinking about how we are able to blend this idea of 672 01:00:28,860 --> 01:00:34,530 economics and ecology in scaling these two things so that different products are operating on different time scales. 673 01:00:34,530 --> 01:00:38,640 You've got to think about how long you run your program for, and that could be many years. 674 01:00:38,730 --> 01:00:42,090 But these mosquitoes and people getting sick is on a daily basis. 675 01:00:42,090 --> 01:00:46,860 And as I said, many, many people still die from from from these sorts of infectious diseases. 676 01:00:47,610 --> 01:00:55,919 Okay. I I'm just about done. I think I've told you about three things, three ways that we can scale slow, fast dynamics. 677 01:00:55,920 --> 01:01:01,469 I didn't do the scaling laws that I might have expected to do, but I've done some dynamical stuff, and I hope you found it interesting. 678 01:01:01,470 --> 01:01:04,620 We can have slow, fast dynamics so we can set those things, one, 679 01:01:04,620 --> 01:01:09,780 going fast and the other we can make really cool approximations and use some very, very cool master work that through. 680 01:01:10,140 --> 01:01:17,370 We worked on the functional response for enterprise interactions in particular, and there's a ways in which we can approximate these things with, 681 01:01:18,360 --> 01:01:26,400 with particular types of functions like the Taylor series, there's really nice ways and think linking things that operate on a different time scales. 682 01:01:27,090 --> 01:01:31,229 We do a raft of things in my group, if anyone is interested. We have a great website, some of them around the room. 683 01:01:31,230 --> 01:01:33,720 If they stick their hands up, they may show their faces, they may not. 684 01:01:33,900 --> 01:01:37,740 They may run out the room thinking, Oh my God, my supervisor is embarrassing me, but that's perfectly fine. 685 01:01:38,160 --> 01:01:41,220 And I'm more than happy to take some questions. Thank you.